Media Releases

Kids praised for being smart are more likely to cheat, new studies find

September 12, 2017

Toron­to, ON – Kids who are praised for being smart, or who are told they have a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart, are more like­ly to be dis­hon­est and cheat, a pair of stud­ies from the Ontario Insti­tute for Stud­ies in Edu­ca­tion (OISE) at the Uni­ver­si­ty of Toron­to and researchers in the U.S. and Chi­na has found.

OISE’s Jack­man Insti­tute of Child Stud­ies (JICS) Pro­fes­sor Kang Lee and study co-authors say that while praise is one of the most com­mon­ly used forms of reward by par­ents and edu­ca­tors around the world, stud­ies show that when used incor­rect­ly, it can back­fire: “Giv­ing chil­dren wrong kind of praise makes them dis­hon­est,” said Pro­fes­sor Lee.

‘Smart’ ver­sus ‘doing great’

In the first study, “Prais­ing young chil­dren for being smart pro­motes cheat­ing”, pub­lished today in Psy­cho­log­i­cal Sci­ence, preschool­ers who were praised for being smart were more like­ly to cheat sub­se­quent­ly than those who were praised for doing “great” in a par­tic­u­lar task. Sim­i­lar­ly, in the sec­ond study,“Telling young chil­dren they have a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart pro­motes cheat­ing”, recent­ly pub­lished in Devel­op­men­tal Sci­ence, preschool­ers who were told that they had a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart also became more like­ly to cheat.

In first study, researchers asked three and five-year-olds to play a guess­ing game. When chil­dren did well in one occa­sion they were praised in one of two ways: one half of the chil­dren were praised for being smart (i.e., “You are so smart.”), while the oth­er half were praised for their per­for­mance (i.e., “You did very well this time.”). After receiv­ing either type of praise, the chil­dren con­tin­ued to play the guess­ing games. Researchers then left the room after ask­ing chil­dren to promise not to cheat by peek­ing at the answers. Their behav­iour was then mon­i­tored by a hid­den cam­era.

Results show that despite the sub­tle dif­fer­ence between the two forms of praise, the chil­dren who were praised for being smart were more like­ly to act dis­hon­est­ly than the chil­dren who had been praised for their behav­iour in a spe­cif­ic game. Results were the same for both ages.

In the sec­ond study, researchers told each child that he or she had a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart. Hear­ing this, sim­i­lar­ly to receiv­ing direct “smart­ness” praise, also had the effect of increas­ing chil­dren’s ten­den­cy to cheat.

‘Abil­i­ty praise’ seen as linked to sta­ble traits

Why is it that giv­ing chil­dren praise for being smart pro­motes dis­hon­esty?

“Praise is more com­plex than it seems,” said Pro­fes­sor Lee. “Prais­ing a child’s abil­i­ty implies that the spe­cif­ic behav­iour that is com­ment­ed on stems from sta­ble traits relat­ed to one’s abil­i­ty, such as smart­ness. This is dif­fer­ent than oth­er forms of praise, such as prais­ing spe­cif­ic behav­iours or prais­ing effort.”

Not­ing pre­vi­ous research which shows abil­i­ty praise can under­mine a child’s moti­va­tion to learn when they encounter dif­fi­cul­ties, Uni­ver­si­ty of Cal­i­for­nia San Diego Pro­fes­sor Gail Hey­man, co-author of the stud­ies, find­ings show that the neg­a­tive effects of abil­i­ty praise extend beyond this to pro­mot­ing dis­hon­esty, and that this occurs in chil­dren as young as three years of age.”

Being ‘smart’ cre­ates expec­ta­tions

Hangzhou Nor­mal University’s Pro­fes­sor Li Zhao, also co-author of the stud­ies, explained that when chil­dren are praised for being smart or learn that they have a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart, “they feel pres­sure to per­form well in order to live up to oth­ers’ expec­ta­tions, even if they need to cheat to do so.” She fur­ther explained that prais­ing a child’s spe­cif­ic behav­iour does not imply that the child is expect­ed to con­sis­tent­ly per­form well and there­fore does not have sim­i­lar neg­a­tive effects as abil­i­ty praise.

Over­all, Pro­fes­sor Lee said for adults, the stud­ies show the impor­tance of learn­ing to praise in a way that doesn’t prompt or pro­mote dis­hon­est behav­iour.

“We want to encour­age chil­dren, we want them to feel good about them­selves. But these stud­ies show we must learn to give chil­dren the right kinds of praise, such as prais­ing spe­cif­ic behav­iour. Only in this way, will praise have the intend­ed pos­i­tive out­comes.”

View the stud­ies:

“Prais­ing young chil­dren for being smart pro­motes cheat­ing”

“Telling young chil­dren they have a rep­u­ta­tion for being smart pro­motes cheat­ing”

Con­tact

Dr. Kang Lee, Pro­fes­sor, Eric Jack­man Insti­tute of Child Study (JICS), Ontario Insti­tute for Stud­ies in Edu­ca­tion (OISE), Uni­ver­si­ty of Toron­to
Email: kang.lee@utoronto.ca   Phone: 647–606-6849

Dr. Gail Hey­man, Pro­fes­sor, Depart­ment of Psy­chol­o­gy, Uni­ver­si­ty of Cal­i­for­nia San Die­goE­mail: gheyman@ucsd.edu

Dr. Li Zhao, Asso­ciate Pro­fes­sor, Insti­tutes of Psy­cho­log­i­cal Sci­ences, Hangzhou Nor­mal Uni­ver­si­ty, P.R. Chi­na
Email: zhaoli1983830@126.com

Lind­sey Craig, Media Rela­tions Coor­di­na­tor, Ontario Insti­tute for Stud­ies in Edu­ca­tion (OISE)Email: lindsey.craig@utoronto.ca   Phone: 416–978-1127